Cognitively Stimulating Activities: Effects on Cognition across Four Studies with up to 21 Years of Longitudinal Data

Engagement in cognitively stimulating activities has been considered to maintain or strengthen cognitive skills, thereby minimizing age-related cognitive decline. While the idea that there may be a modifiable behavior that could lower risk for cognitive decline is appealing and potentially empowerin...

Full description

Saved in:
Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Meghan B. Mitchell, Cynthia R. Cimino, Andreana Benitez, Cassandra L. Brown, Laura E. Gibbons, Robert F. Kennison, Steven D. Shirk, Alireza Atri, Annie Robitaille, Stuart W. S. MacDonald, Magnus Lindwall, Elizabeth M. Zelinski, Sherry L. Willis, K. Warner Schaie, Boo Johansson, Roger A. Dixon, Dan M. Mungas, Scott M. Hofer, Andrea M. Piccinin
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Wiley 2012-01-01
Series:Journal of Aging Research
Online Access:http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/461592
Tags: Add Tag
No Tags, Be the first to tag this record!
_version_ 1832562863995092992
author Meghan B. Mitchell
Cynthia R. Cimino
Andreana Benitez
Cassandra L. Brown
Laura E. Gibbons
Robert F. Kennison
Steven D. Shirk
Alireza Atri
Annie Robitaille
Stuart W. S. MacDonald
Magnus Lindwall
Elizabeth M. Zelinski
Sherry L. Willis
K. Warner Schaie
Boo Johansson
Roger A. Dixon
Dan M. Mungas
Scott M. Hofer
Andrea M. Piccinin
author_facet Meghan B. Mitchell
Cynthia R. Cimino
Andreana Benitez
Cassandra L. Brown
Laura E. Gibbons
Robert F. Kennison
Steven D. Shirk
Alireza Atri
Annie Robitaille
Stuart W. S. MacDonald
Magnus Lindwall
Elizabeth M. Zelinski
Sherry L. Willis
K. Warner Schaie
Boo Johansson
Roger A. Dixon
Dan M. Mungas
Scott M. Hofer
Andrea M. Piccinin
author_sort Meghan B. Mitchell
collection DOAJ
description Engagement in cognitively stimulating activities has been considered to maintain or strengthen cognitive skills, thereby minimizing age-related cognitive decline. While the idea that there may be a modifiable behavior that could lower risk for cognitive decline is appealing and potentially empowering for older adults, research findings have not consistently supported the beneficial effects of engaging in cognitively stimulating tasks. Using observational studies of naturalistic cognitive activities, we report a series of mixed effects models that include baseline and change in cognitive activity predicting cognitive outcomes over up to 21 years in four longitudinal studies of aging. Consistent evidence was found for cross-sectional relationships between level of cognitive activity and cognitive test performance. Baseline activity at an earlier age did not, however, predict rate of decline later in life, thus not supporting the concept that engaging in cognitive activity at an earlier point in time increases one's ability to mitigate future age-related cognitive decline. In contrast, change in activity was associated with relative change in cognitive performance. Results therefore suggest that change in cognitive activity from one's previous level has at least a transitory association with cognitive performance measured at the same point in time.
format Article
id doaj-art-3240cea427ba4158ad21970d5ab2e292
institution Kabale University
issn 2090-2204
2090-2212
language English
publishDate 2012-01-01
publisher Wiley
record_format Article
series Journal of Aging Research
spelling doaj-art-3240cea427ba4158ad21970d5ab2e2922025-02-03T01:21:33ZengWileyJournal of Aging Research2090-22042090-22122012-01-01201210.1155/2012/461592461592Cognitively Stimulating Activities: Effects on Cognition across Four Studies with up to 21 Years of Longitudinal DataMeghan B. Mitchell0Cynthia R. Cimino1Andreana Benitez2Cassandra L. Brown3Laura E. Gibbons4Robert F. Kennison5Steven D. Shirk6Alireza Atri7Annie Robitaille8Stuart W. S. MacDonald9Magnus Lindwall10Elizabeth M. Zelinski11Sherry L. Willis12K. Warner Schaie13Boo Johansson14Roger A. Dixon15Dan M. Mungas16Scott M. Hofer17Andrea M. Piccinin18Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Geriatric Research, Education and Clinical Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 200 Springs Road, Bedford, MA 01730, USADepartments of Psychology and Department of Neurology, University of South Florida, 4202 E. Fowler Avenue, Tampa, FL 33620, USACenter for Biomedical Imaging, Medical University of South Carolina, 68 President Street, MSC 120, Charleston, SC 29425, USADepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, P.O. Box 3050 STN CSC, Victoria, BC, V8W 3P5, CanadaDivision of General Internal Medicine, University of Washington, P.O. Box 359780, Harborview Medical Center, 325 Ninth Avenue Seattle, WA 98104, USADepartment of Psychology, California State University-Los Angeles 5151 State University Drive, Los Angeles, CA 90032, USAEdith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Geriatric Research, Education and Clinical Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 200 Springs Road, Bedford, MA 01730, USAEdith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Geriatric Research, Education and Clinical Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, 200 Springs Road, Bedford, MA 01730, USADepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, P.O. Box 3050 STN CSC, Victoria, BC, V8W 3P5, CanadaDepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, P.O. Box 3050 STN CSC, Victoria, BC, V8W 3P5, CanadaDepartment of Food and Nutrition, and Sport Science, Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, P.O. Box 100, SE-405 30 Gothenburg, SwedenEthel Percy Andrus Gerontology Center, Leonard Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-0191, USAUniversity of Washington, 180 Nickerson, Suite 206, Seattle, WA 98109, USADivision of General Internal Medicine, University of Washington, P.O. Box 359780, Harborview Medical Center, 325 Ninth Avenue Seattle, WA 98104, USADepartment of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Box 500, SE 405 30, Gothenburg, SwedenDepartment of Psychology, University of Alberta, P217 Biological Sciences Building, Edmonton, AB, T6G 2E9, CanadaLawrence J. Ellison Ambulatory Care Center, University of California, Davis, 4860 Y Street, Ste 0100, Sacramento, CA 95817, USADepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, P.O. Box 3050 STN CSC, Victoria, BC, V8W 3P5, CanadaDepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, P.O. Box 3050 STN CSC, Victoria, BC, V8W 3P5, CanadaEngagement in cognitively stimulating activities has been considered to maintain or strengthen cognitive skills, thereby minimizing age-related cognitive decline. While the idea that there may be a modifiable behavior that could lower risk for cognitive decline is appealing and potentially empowering for older adults, research findings have not consistently supported the beneficial effects of engaging in cognitively stimulating tasks. Using observational studies of naturalistic cognitive activities, we report a series of mixed effects models that include baseline and change in cognitive activity predicting cognitive outcomes over up to 21 years in four longitudinal studies of aging. Consistent evidence was found for cross-sectional relationships between level of cognitive activity and cognitive test performance. Baseline activity at an earlier age did not, however, predict rate of decline later in life, thus not supporting the concept that engaging in cognitive activity at an earlier point in time increases one's ability to mitigate future age-related cognitive decline. In contrast, change in activity was associated with relative change in cognitive performance. Results therefore suggest that change in cognitive activity from one's previous level has at least a transitory association with cognitive performance measured at the same point in time.http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/461592
spellingShingle Meghan B. Mitchell
Cynthia R. Cimino
Andreana Benitez
Cassandra L. Brown
Laura E. Gibbons
Robert F. Kennison
Steven D. Shirk
Alireza Atri
Annie Robitaille
Stuart W. S. MacDonald
Magnus Lindwall
Elizabeth M. Zelinski
Sherry L. Willis
K. Warner Schaie
Boo Johansson
Roger A. Dixon
Dan M. Mungas
Scott M. Hofer
Andrea M. Piccinin
Cognitively Stimulating Activities: Effects on Cognition across Four Studies with up to 21 Years of Longitudinal Data
Journal of Aging Research
title Cognitively Stimulating Activities: Effects on Cognition across Four Studies with up to 21 Years of Longitudinal Data
title_full Cognitively Stimulating Activities: Effects on Cognition across Four Studies with up to 21 Years of Longitudinal Data
title_fullStr Cognitively Stimulating Activities: Effects on Cognition across Four Studies with up to 21 Years of Longitudinal Data
title_full_unstemmed Cognitively Stimulating Activities: Effects on Cognition across Four Studies with up to 21 Years of Longitudinal Data
title_short Cognitively Stimulating Activities: Effects on Cognition across Four Studies with up to 21 Years of Longitudinal Data
title_sort cognitively stimulating activities effects on cognition across four studies with up to 21 years of longitudinal data
url http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/461592
work_keys_str_mv AT meghanbmitchell cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT cynthiarcimino cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT andreanabenitez cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT cassandralbrown cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT lauraegibbons cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT robertfkennison cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT stevendshirk cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT alirezaatri cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT annierobitaille cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT stuartwsmacdonald cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT magnuslindwall cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT elizabethmzelinski cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT sherrylwillis cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT kwarnerschaie cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT boojohansson cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT rogeradixon cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT danmmungas cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT scottmhofer cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata
AT andreampiccinin cognitivelystimulatingactivitieseffectsoncognitionacrossfourstudieswithupto21yearsoflongitudinaldata